477 Facebook posts from our supplemental Facebook group to WVU COM105 "Introduction to the Mass Media" |
To help address some of these points, our research team of myself, Bryand and WVU undergraduate Lindsey Carr wondered using Facebook as supplemental space to the mass lecture might be a useful and cost-effective way to enhance the mass lecture experience. Thus, in Fall 2011 students (N = 321, 195 male and 126 female) enrolled in a mass lecture were invited to join a closed Facebook group devoted to their class. Participation in the group was strictly voluntary and students were assured that their profile information would remain private and closed to other members (or at least, they would not be required to friend anyone to use the page). We wondered if involvement in the group might influence learning and attendance in class, and the results are quite interesting. [NOTE: Data analysis still in progress, particularly with affective reactions to class]
DESCRIPTIVES:
Course grade. Three exams were given in COM105 to provide an assessment of student’s basic understanding of course concepts; our cognitive assessment. These exams were 50-item, four-response options closed-ended format, with slightly heterogenous test items (average Kuder-Richardson 20 = .76) falling just below the KR = .80 cut-off suggested by (Lewis & Ortiz, 1988). All three were weighted equally; these grades were added together and transformed into a simple percentage, with students scoring an average grade of M = 75.27%, SD = 12.57.
Attendance. Attendance in COM105 was measured using a random assignment of seven different attendance activities given throughout the semester; this was considered a measure of student engagement. These activities were coded “1” (completed) or “0” (not completed), and students completed an average of M = 4.80 of these assignments, SD = 1.98. Research activity (extra credit). Students were provided with three opportunities to earn six total extra credit points in COM105, and these scores were tracked as an indicator of increased engagement in the course. Students completed an average of M = 3.55 of these six points, SD = 1.61.
Use of Facebook group. In all, n = 142 students (44%) used the Facebook group, posting an average of M = 6.88 posts, SD = 9.50. Students posted an average of Knowing that a significant bias exists such that females are more likely to use social media than males (Bowman,Westerman, & Claus, 2012), we examined the distribution of Facebook members and non-members by gender; no significant difference was found, χ2 (1) = .043, p = .835. For both males and females, distribution to Facebook members and non-members was nearly 50% each (males: 89 members, 106 non-members; females: 59 members, 67 non-members). A total of 477 posts were made to the Facebook students, with 283 of these (~ 60%) posted by students. Cluster analysis by three expert coders (i.e., authors of this report) found nine distinct categories of post, see Figure below.
Exam Review | Admin | Class-Related External Links | Peer Support | Humor | Affect for Instructor/Class | Unrelated External Links | Instructor Support | Unclassified | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
# posts | 201 | 119 | 61 | 17 | 16 | 15 | 13 | 8 | 27 |
Avg # comments per post | 3.60 | 2.47 | 1.46 | 3.41 | 2.74 | .292 | .288 | .375 | 1.64 |
# posts initiated by instructor | 64 | 60 | 39 | 0 | 3 | 1 | 5 | 8 | 13 |
Avg # of comments per post | 3.66 | 1.22 | .923 | 0 | 4.33 | 0 | 0 | .375 | 2.92 |
# posts initiated by students | 137 | 59 | 22 | 17 | 13 | 14 | 8 | 0 | 14 |
Avg # of comments per post | 3.54 | 3.71 | 2.00 | 3.41 | 1.15 | 4.39 | 3.75 | 0 | .357 |
Unsurprisingly given visual inspection, a chi-square goodness-of-fit test reveals a significantly different distribution of comments by comment type, χ2 (8) = .655.3, p < .001. Comparing to an expected distribution of 53 comments per category (i.e., a null effect of comment category on observed comments) we see that exam review and administrative questions were by far the most prominently-posted comments, with the other seven categories falling under what might be expected by chance. Comparing these two categories against each other, we see a significant difference in that exam review questions occurred far greater than change, χ2 (1) = 21.01, p < .001). A few patterns in here are interesting, and deserve some further investigation, mainly (and we note the purely descriptive nature of this analysis; in fact we are open to analysis suggestions as we work this data set:
- Students initiated exam review questions at a nearly 2:1 ratio as compared to instructor-initiated exam review posts, yet the number of comments generated did not differ from each other. This suggests students on the Facebook page to be proactive in their studying.
- Students engaged in posts related to general humor as well as expressing support for each other and the professor of the class. of the 477 posts, only one was found that expressed dissent with the course (a student upset about an exam being perceived as too difficult).
- What else are you seeing in here?
RESULTS:
Course engagement. Students who were members of the class Facebook group submitted more attendance assignments (M = 5.23, SD = 1.79) than students who were not in the group (M = 4.43, SD = 2.07), t(319) = -3.69, p < .001. No significant difference was found between the groups for completing extra credit assignments, t(319) = 1.70, p = .571; both groups completed an average of M = 3.55 extra points, SD = 1.61. For students in the Facebook group, they posted an average of M = 6.88 post, SD = 9.05.
Cognitive assessment. Students who were members of the class Facebook group scored significantly higher in the class (M = 78.55%, SD = 8.54) than students who were not in the group (M = 72.64%, SD = 13.60), t(319) = -4.71, p < 001. For students in the Facebook group, there was no significant association between the number of times an individual posted in the Facebook group and their attendance (r = .124, p = .144) or their course grade (r = .158, p = .061). This data suggest that membership in the group was the primary influence on the witnessed effects rather than increased active involvement in the group. Some support for this finding can be found in looking at the distribution analyses of Facebook posts per user, which showed a heavy positive and leptokurdic skew (skewness = 4.09, kurtosis = 26.23), suggesting a few super-users providing the majority of Facebook content with many users lurking from this.
So, there you have it. Early, quick and dirty analysis -but something to get the conversation going! Some very early and preliminary data to suggest that encouraging students to use Facebook to supplement the mass lecture can be an effective way to get them engaged in class and, if you can get them posting, to enhance their learning of course material. We of course acknowledge limitations of the current study, including a rather base approach to cognitive learning qua exam grades and a potential self-selection bias such that engaged students are more likely to join a class-related Facebook than non-engaged students - both should be investigated further in replication. Yet, we feel strongly that our data suggest that using Facebook as a persistent and cost-effective supplemental space for which to continue discussions in the university mass lecture can significantly enhance student learning. And we look forward to your comments!
No comments:
Post a Comment